0 položky
(+359) 877750542 info@xenobg.com

В миналото писах няколко статии за алтруизма на растенията, както и за тяхното видимо съзнание (повлияно от анестезия), въпреки че нямат нервна система. В статията по-долу са описани допълнителни експерименти, които показват, че растенията очевидно са способни на редица зашеметяващи подвизи, които ги правят да изглеждат съвсем човешки, а в някои случаи и свръхчовешки. Очевидно растенията могат (наред с много други неща) да „четат човешки мисли“, да „усещат стреса“, да запомнят и да се адаптират към опасно събитие/среда и дори да „разпознаят“ растение убиец. Аз лично не познавам нито един човек, който да е способен на първия и последния от тези подвизи, но това не пречи на голям брой хора да твърдят, че могат, и да взимат доста пари за „услугата“. Като се има предвид, че Чарлз Дарвин, повечето други негови съвременни биолози и дори древните гърци твърдо са вярвали, че растенията притежават интелект, не много по-различен от нашия, наличието на интелект у всяко живо същество може да се окаже поредната „спорна“ (разбирайте: вярна и застрашаваща печалбите на някоя индустрия) идея, която е била целенасочено задушавана през тъмните векове на науката (например през XX век). Както се казва в статията, последиците от подобно невежество и/или задушаване са дълбоки. Ако растенията притежават интелигентност, може да се наложи самата ни връзка с реалността и представата за нея да бъдат разрушени и изградени отначало.

http://nautil.us/issue/77/underworlds/never-underestimate-the-intelligence-of-trees

https://www.theparisreview.org/blog/2019/09/26/the-intelligence-of-plants

“…A few years ago, Monica Gagliano, an associate professor in evolutionary ecology at the University of Western Australia, began dropping potted Mimosa pudicas. She used a sliding steel rail that guided them to six inches above a cushioned surface, then let them fall. The plant, which is leafy and green with pink-purple flower heads, is commonly known as a “shameplant” or a “touch-me-not” because its leaves fold inward when it’s disturbed. In theory, it would defend itself against any attack, indiscriminately perceiving any touch or drop as an offense and closing itself up. The first time Gagliano dropped the plants—fifty-six of them—from the measured height, they responded as expected. But after several more drops, fewer of them closed. She dropped each of them sixty times, in five-second intervals. Eventually, all of them stopped closing. She continued like this for twenty-eight days, but none of them ever closed up again. It was only when she bothered them differently—such as by grabbing them—that they reverted to their usual defense mechanism. Gagliano concluded, in a study published in a 2014 edition of Oecologia, that the shameplants had “remembered” that their being dropped from such a low height wasn’t actually a danger and realized they didn’t need to defend themselves. She believed that her experiment helped prove that “brains and neurons are a sophisticated solution but not a necessary requirement for learning.” The plants, she reasoned, were learning. The plants, she believed, were remembering. Bees, for instance, forget what they’ve learned after just a few days. These shameplants had remembered for nearly a month.”

“…The idea of a “plant intelligence”—an intelligence that goes beyond adaptation and reaction and into the realm of active memory and decision-making—has been in the air since at least the early seventies. A shift from religion to “spirituality” in the sixties and seventies unlocked new avenues of belief, and the 1973 bestseller The Secret Life of Plants catalyzed the phenomenon. Written by Peter Tompkins and Christopher Bird, the book made some claims, such as that plants can “read human minds,” “feel stress,” and “pick out” a plant murderer. Mostly, it proved to be a touchstone. Cleve Backster, a polygraph tester for the CIA, did one such experiment in 1966 when, “on a whim,” he attached a galvanometer (a machine that registers electrical currents) to a dracaena, a tropical palm houseplant. Silently, Backster imagined the plant was on fire. The galvanometer flickered. Backster concluded the plant was feeling stress from his thoughts. “Could the plant have been reading his mind?” ask Tompkins and Bird in the book. In another experiment, Backster had a friend stomp on a plant. Then, that friend and five other human “suspects” walked out in front of the plant that had “witnessed” the stomping. The plant was hooked up to a galvanometer. When the killer entered the room, the plant sent out a wave of electricity, thereby “identifying” the murderer.”

“…Darwin floated the first modern ideation of plant intelligence in 1880. Writing in The Power of Movement in Plantshe concluded that the root of a plant has “the power of directing the movements of the adjoining parts” and thus “acts like the brain of one of the lower animals; the brain being seated within the anterior end of the body, receiving impressions from the sense organs and directing the several movements.” Darwin was talking about how plants react to shifts in vibrations, sounds, touch, humidity, and temperature—but these are just adaptive reactionsFacing toward the sun or closing at a touch does not require quasi-neurological abilities. There is no processing or choice involved—unlike the apparent memory shown by Gagliano’s experiment. (Many of the Ancient Greeks—like Plato, Anaxagoras, Democritus, and Empedocles—shared the belief that plants have a kind of brain where sensitivities could be “processed.”)”

“…When Wohlleben came across a tree stump that had been felled probably half a millennium ago, he realized—scraping at it and seeing that it was still bright green beneath—that the trees around it had been keeping it alive, sending it glucose and other nutrients. This plant communications system works similarly to the nervous system of animals. Trees can send out electrical pulse signals underground as well as signals through the air, via pheromones and gasses. When an animal, for instance, begins chewing on a tree’s leaves, the tree can release ethylene gas into the soil, alerting other trees, whereupon those nearby trees can send tannins into their leaves so that if they, too, get their leaves chewed, they might be able to poison the offending animal.”

“…Trees can differentiate between threats, as well. They respond differently to a human breaking off one of its branches than they do to an animal eating at them—with the former, it will try to heal; with the latter, it will try to poison. Plants even share space with one another. In a 2010 studywhen four Cakile edentula, or “sea-rocket plants,” were put in the same pot, they shared their resources, moving their roots to accommodate the others. If the plants were just acting evolutionarily, it would follow that they would compete for resources; instead, they seem to be “thinking” of the other plants and “deciding” to help them. Even the slightest possibility of a proven plant intelligence would have massive scientific and existential implications. If plants can “learn” and “remember,” as Gagliano believes, then humans may have been misunderstanding plants, and ourselves, for all of history. The common understanding of “intelligence” would have to be reimagined; and we’d have missed an entire universe of thought happening all around us.”

“…Plants, as Gagliano concluded, might have a far greater sentience than we’d ever thought possible. The implications of plants that “remember” being dropped and “decide” it’s safe to not protect themselves aren’t best expressed in anthropomorphic terms, but we don’t yet have other language. In truth, we know so little even about ourselves; our science cannot fully explain how humans learn and remember. Why not consider that plants have been doing the same for far longer than we have been around, with an intelligence that is radically different from ours?”

Източник:

http://haidut.me/?p=648

cs_CZ
Усети, мисли, действай
Přehled ochrany osobních údajů

Tyto webové stránky používají soubory cookies, abychom vám mohli poskytnout co nejlepší uživatelský zážitek. Informace o souborech cookie se ukládají ve vašem prohlížeči a plní funkce, jako je rozpoznání, když se na naše webové stránky vrátíte, a pomáhají našemu týmu pochopit, které části webových stránek považujete za nejzajímavější a nejužitečnější.